Precocial
and Altricial Young
"Precocial"
and "altricial," two words describing the degree of
development in young birds at hatching, are good examples of
useful scientific jargon. They save ornithologists from
repeatedly using phrases when single words will do. A
precocial bird is "capable of moving around on its own soon
after hatching." The word comes from the same Latin root as
"precocious." Altricial means "incapable of moving around on
its own soon after hatchling." It comes from a Latin root
meaning "to nourish" a reference to the need for extensive
parental care required before fledging in altricial species.
If you consult some of the literature we have cited, you may
sometimes see the term "nidifugous" used to describe
precocial young that leave the nest immediately, and
"nidicolous" to describe young that remain in the nest. All
nidifugous birds are precocial, but some nidicolous birds
are precocial, too-they remain in the nest even though
capable of locomotion. These terms are less widely used than
precocial and altricial, and we will not employ them outside
of this essay.
Instead of a sharp dividing
line between hatchlings that are precocial and those that
are altricial, there is a gradient of precociality. In this
guide, we recognize the following categories of
young:
Precocial
Hatched with eyes open, covered with down, and leave the
nest within two days. There are four levels of
precociality, although only three are found in North
American birds. Level 1 of development (precocial 1) is
the pattern found in the chicks of megapodes (Australian
Malee fowl, Brush Turkeys, etc.), which are totally
independent of their parents. The megapode young are
incubated in huge piles of decaying vegetation, and upon
hatching dig their way out, already well feathered and
able to fly. No North American birds show this extreme
precociality. Precocial 2 development is found in
ducklings and the chicks of shorebirds, which follow
their parents but find their own food. The young of game
birds, however, trail after their parents and are shown
food; they are classified as precocial 3. Precocial 4
development is represented by the young of birds such as
rails and grebes, which follow their parents and are not
just shown food but are actually fed by them.
Semi-precocial
Hatched with eyes
open, covered with down, and capable of leaving the nest
soon after hatching (they can walk and often swim), but
stay at the nest and are fed by parents. Basically
precocial but nidicolus, this developmental pattern is
found in the young of gulls and terns.
Semi-altricial
Covered with down, incapable of departing from the nest,
and fed by the parents. In species classified as
semi-altricial 1, such as hawks and herons, chicks hatch
with their eyes open. Owls, in the category
semi-altricial 2, hatch with the eyes closed. If all
young were divided into only two categories, altricial
and precocial, these all would be considered
altricial
Altricial
Hatched with eyes closed, with little or no down,
incapable of departing from the nest, and fed by the
parents. All passerines are altricial.
Note that in the species
treatments in this book we use the term "fledging" (F:) for
the number of days it takes for the young of an altricial or
semi-altricial bird to acquire its full set of feathers,
after which it leaves the nest. Thus for altricial and
semi-altricial birds, the time needed to get fully feathered
and time spent in the nest are essentially the same. In
precocial and semi-precocial birds, F: indicates not the
number of days that pass before the young leave the nest,
but the time from hatching until they can fly.
Left: House Sparrow
hatchling (altricial-naked, blind and helpless on
hatching).
Right: Ruffed Grouse hatchling (precocial 3-downy,
open-eyed, mobile on hatching, follows parents and
is shown food).
|
Characteristics Of
Nestlings
(modified
from O'Connor, 1984)
TYPE
OF
DEVELOPMENT
|
DOWN
PRESENT?
|
EYES
OPEN?
|
MOBILE?.
|
FEED.
SELVES?
|
PARENTS
ABSENT?
|
EXAMPLES
|
Precocial
1
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Megapodes
|
Precocial
2
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes*
|
No
|
Ducks,
Plovers
|
Precocial
3
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
No
|
Quail,
Turkey
|
Precocial
4
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes/No
|
No
|
Grebes,
Rails
|
Semi-precocial
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes/No
|
No
|
No
|
Gulls,
Terns
|
Semi-altricial
1
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
Herons,
Hawks
|
Semi-altricial
2
|
Yes
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
Owls
|
Altricial
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
No
|
Passerines
|
...
t =
Precocial 3 are shown food.
.....*=
Precocial 2 follow parents but find own food.
Why have these different modes of development evolved? They
are obviously tied into two important aspects of the bird's
environment: food availability and predation pressure.
Precociality puts a premium on the ability of females to
obtain abundant resources before laying. They must produce
energy-rich eggs to support the greater in-egg development
of the chicks (eggs of precocial birds may contain almost
twice the calories per unit weight as those of altricial
birds). Females of altricial species do not have such large
nutritional demands before egg laying, but must be able
(with their mates) to find sufficient food to rush their
helpless young through to fledging. While the young are in
the nest, the entire brood is extremely vulnerable to
predation and is dependent on concealment of the nest and
parental defense for survival. In contrast, precocial young,
having left the nest, have some ability to avoid predation,
and there is a much smaller chance of the entire brood (as
opposed to single chicks) being devoured.
Interestingly, there seems
to be an evolutionary trade-off in bird brain sizes related
to the degree of precocity. Precocial species have
relatively large brains at hatching-as one might expect
since the young, to one degree or another, must be able to
fend for themselves. But precocial species trade for this
advantage an adult brain that is small in relation to their
body size. Altricial young, in contrast, are born
small-brained, but on the pro-tern-rich diet provided by the
adults (and with their highly efficient digestive tracts)
postnatal brain growth is great, and the adults have
proportionally larger brains than precocial
species.
Parrots have evolved their
way into the best of both worlds. They are altricial, but
the female invests in a nutrient-rich egg just like females
of precocial species. Parrots are among the most intelligent
of birds; they have adopted the same evolutionary strategy
as we have. People (like other primates, elephants, and
antelopes, but unlike rodents and marsupials) are
precocial-born with hair, open eyes, and large brains. But
our brains and those of parrots, both large at birth, also
grow a great deal after birth as a result of large parental
investments of food energy.
Thus a complex evolutionary
problem of balancing the need to provide nourishment to the
young and to protect them from predation has been
"solved" by each group of
birds-and the solutions are the different avian
developmental patterns we now observe. Similar problems have
been solved, also in diverse ways, in the course of
mammalian evolution. But many more groups of mammals than
birds have managed to become big-brained as both young and
adults.
SEE: Parental
Care.
Copyright
® 1988 by Paul R. Ehrlich, David S. Dobkin, and Darryl
Wheye.
|