Bulging
muscles and jeweled adornments are nonarbitrary symbols in
human society, denoting strength and wealth, respectively.
On the other hand, many arbitrary symbols -- the bishop's
mitre, the admiral's gold braid, the karate master's black
belt, the judge's gown, the knight's title of Sir -- are
also recognized. Such symbols are arbitrary because they
signal status without having any inherent connection with
the status signaled. Nonarbitrary status symbols
are readily found in nonhuman animals, the classic example
being the size of horns in mountain sheep. In those
creatures a small-horned male will avoid a large-homed
stranger seen at a distance, even though the two have never
determined their relative positions in a dominance hierarchy
by actual combat. The existence of arbitrary symbols outside
of human society has been more problematic. British
evolutionists Richard Dawkins and John Krebs adopted the
term "badge" for arbitrary animal symbols, and avian
biologists, especially Sievert Rohwer, have carried out
investigations to see if badges play a role in dominance
relationships in bird flocks. Harris' Sparrows assemble in
flocks of mixed ages in the winter. Individuals within the
flocks show a great deal of variation in their plumage
characteristics, especially in the amount of darkness on
their heads and "bibs." In a series of experiments Rohwer
darkened relatively light first-year birds with dye to make
them look like adults. He found, for example, that dyed
first-year birds are initially avoided by undyed "control"
first-year birds. Then the dyed birds began actively to
dominate the controls. Rohwer thus concluded that the
dominance status of a Harris' Sparrow can be communicated by
a badge, a dark head and bib. The alternate explanation
(that the dye in some way actually enhanced the combat
ability of dyed birds) is too unlikely to merit serious
consideration. Differences in darkness do
not, however, always correlate with status, as behaviorist
Doris Watt has shown. In experiments Watt showed that within
age classes, at least in small groups, darker birds are not
always dominant over lighter individuals. She believes the
darkness to be basically a badge of age, which signals to
first-year birds the potential dominance of adults.
Apparently among both young and adults, variation in breast
spot patterns (as opposed to overall breast darkness) aids
individuals in recognizing one another, but does not
indicate status. Status signals may allow
associations to form between dominant and subordinate
individuals so that both may benefit, as Rohwer and his
colleague, Paul Ewald, have suggested. Subordinates, for
instance, may learn what foods are most nutritious, while
dominants may be able to place subordinates between
themselves and potential predators. Badges also exist in
White-crowned Sparrows, as Gary Fugle and his coworkers at
the University of California have demonstrated. Adult males,
which have bright black and white striping on the head, have
the highest dominance status, juvenile females with dull
striping the lowest, and adult females with intermediate
striping intermediate status. Experiments in which the heads
of juvenile and adult females were painted to resemble adult
males revealed that brighter striping added to the status of
the painted birds relative to unpainted controls. Similarly,
experiments with stuffed birds (mounts) indicate that a male
Yellow Warbler signals his status, in particular his level
of aggressive motivation, by the amount of brown streaking
on his breast. Brighter (more streaked) mounts, elicited
more aggressive responses from males than duller mounts,
while brighter males responded more aggressively to mounts
than did duller males. Perhaps the most ingenious
experiment on avian badges involved the European Great Tit,
a close relative of North American chickadees and titmice
(all are members of the same genus, Parus). This species has
a dark cap, white cheeks, and a dark breast stripe/bib.
Torbiorn Järvi and Marten Bakken of Norway's University
of Trondheim used radio-controlled motorized stuffed birds
at a feeder to test the efficacy of the breast stripe as a
badge. When a Great Tit approached the feeder, the stuffed
bird could be rotated to face the incoming individual and to
perform a "head-up" aggressive display. When, and only when,
the stuffed bird had a breast stripe wider than that of the
incoming bird was the latter frightened away. Evidently badges can signal
status in avian societies -- but this presents evolutionists
with a considerable mystery. Since they are arbitrary
symbols, why isn't cheating widespread? Why don't first-year
Harris' Sparrows grow dark plumage just like adults? Why
don't weakling Great Tits develop broad breast stripes and
monopolize the food at feeders? Badge systems seem to have
built into them the seeds of their own evolutionary
destruction, since frequent cheating should soon make all
the signals ambiguous. One way out of this seeming
dilemma has been suggested. Perhaps dominance in monogamous
birds (as opposed to polygynous ones, where dominant males
may get many more matings) does not in itself confer a
selective advantage. Subordinate birds may not get access to
the best resources, but they also may not have to expend
much energy defending what they've got. Being subordinate
may be just as good an evolutionary strategy as being
dominant -- both kinds of individual may be equally
successful reproductively. If that is the case, there would
be no advantage to cheating, and one would not expect such
behavior to evolve. SEE: Redwing
Coverable Badges;
Visual
Displays;
Sexual
Selection;
Dominance
Hierarchies. Copyright
® 1988 by Paul R. Ehrlich, David S. Dobkin, and Darryl
Wheye.